Limits on the Perception of Local Shape from Shading

Similar documents
Perception of local shape from shading

COMP 546. Lecture 11. Shape from X: perspective, texture, shading. Thurs. Feb. 15, 2018

The perception of surface orientation from multiple sources of optical information

Perceived shininess and rigidity - Measurements of shape-dependent specular flow of rotating objects

Perspective and vanishing points

The effects of smooth occlusions and directions of illumination on the visual perception of 3-D shape from shading

Shape from shading: estimation of reflectance map

Local qualitative shape from stereo. without detailed correspondence. Extended Abstract. Shimon Edelman. Internet:

Max Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik. Spemannstraße Tübingen Germany

The Assumed Light Direction for Perceiving Shape from Shading

Conveying Shape with Texture: an experimental investigation of the impact of texture type on shape categorization judgments

Shape Constancy and Shape Recovery: wherein Human and Computer Vision Meet

The perception of surface orientation from multiple sources of optical information

Comment on Numerical shape from shading and occluding boundaries

Representing 3D Objects: An Introduction to Object Centered and Viewer Centered Models

Using surface markings to enhance accuracy and stability of object perception in graphic displays

Last update: May 4, Vision. CMSC 421: Chapter 24. CMSC 421: Chapter 24 1

Effects of Texture, Illumination and Surface Reflectance on Stereoscopic Shape Perception

Report. Specular Image Structure Modulates the Perception of Three-Dimensional Shape. Scott W.J. Mooney 1, * and Barton L.

Depth. Common Classification Tasks. Example: AlexNet. Another Example: Inception. Another Example: Inception. Depth

Recovering Shape from a Single Image of a Mirrored Surface from Curvature Constraints

Chapter Three: Perceiving and Representing Shape and Depth

Light: Geometric Optics

Quadric Surfaces. Philippe B. Laval. Today KSU. Philippe B. Laval (KSU) Quadric Surfaces Today 1 / 24

General Principles of 3D Image Analysis

Quadric Surfaces. Six basic types of quadric surfaces: ellipsoid. cone. elliptic paraboloid. hyperboloid of one sheet. hyperboloid of two sheets

Quadric Surfaces. Philippe B. Laval. Spring 2012 KSU. Philippe B. Laval (KSU) Quadric Surfaces Spring /

Surface Range and Attitude Probing in Stereoscopically Presented Dynamic Scenes

Vision Research. How shape from contours affects shape from shading. Dejan Todorović. abstract. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Perception of Shape from Shading

3216 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 14, No. 12/December 1997 Knill et al. Geometry of shadows. David C. Knill

Perception, Part 2 Gleitman et al. (2011), Chapter 5

A Simple Vision System

Comparison of HK and SC curvature description methods

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Automatic Lighting Design using a Perceptual Quality Metric

12.6 Cylinders and Quadric Surfaces

Illumination and Reflectance

Announcement. Lighting and Photometric Stereo. Computer Vision I. Surface Reflectance Models. Lambertian (Diffuse) Surface.

DIRECT SHAPE FROM ISOPHOTES

The Intrinsic Geometry of Perceptual Space: Its Metrical, Affine and Projective Properties. Abstract

What is Computer Vision?

Shape-from-shading depends on visual, gravitational, and body-orientation cues

STRUCTURE AND MOTION ESTIMATION FROM DYNAMIC SILHOUETTES UNDER PERSPECTIVE PROJECTION *

Lambertian model of reflectance I: shape from shading and photometric stereo. Ronen Basri Weizmann Institute of Science

PROOF COPY [A-8466] JOA

Anno accademico 2006/2007. Davide Migliore

C3 Numerical methods

Conveying 3D Shape and Depth with Textured and Transparent Surfaces Victoria Interrante

BCC Sphere Transition

OBSERVATIONS Visual Perception of Smoothly Curved Surfaces From Double-Projected Contour Patterns

Curvature Estimation on Smoothed 3-D Meshes

CPSC GLOBAL ILLUMINATION

Relative flattening between velvet and matte 3D shapes: Evidence for similar shape-from-shading computations

Homework 4 Computer Vision CS 4731, Fall 2011 Due Date: Nov. 15, 2011 Total Points: 40

Computer Vision I - Appearance-based Matching and Projective Geometry

This version of the article is subject to the terms of a Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial No-Derivatives license.

Color and Shading. Color. Shapiro and Stockman, Chapter 6. Color and Machine Vision. Color and Perception

A Survey of Light Source Detection Methods

Computational Perception /785

CHAPTER 6 PERCEPTUAL ORGANIZATION BASED ON TEMPORAL DYNAMICS

Simultaneous surface texture classification and illumination tilt angle prediction

Image Formation: Light and Shading. Introduction to Computer Vision CSE 152 Lecture 3

Announcements. Stereo Vision Wrapup & Intro Recognition

Perceiving and representing shape & depth

PERCEIVING DEPTH AND SIZE

Perceived 3D metric (or Euclidean) shape is merely ambiguous, not systematically distorted

Matching. Compare region of image to region of image. Today, simplest kind of matching. Intensities similar.

Feature Tracking and Optical Flow

Feature Lines on Surfaces

And if that 120MP Camera was cool

Constructing a 3D Object Model from Multiple Visual Features

Understanding Visual Performance A Pacific Energy Center Factsheet

Peripheral drift illusion

3-D Interpretation of Imperfect Line Drawings

3D Shape Recovery of Smooth Surfaces: Dropping the Fixed Viewpoint Assumption

then assume that we are given the image of one of these textures captured by a camera at a different (longer) distance and with unknown direction of i

3D Models and Matching

3D Models from Contours: Further Identification of Unexposed Areas

Cylinders and Quadric Surfaces A cylinder is a three dimensional shape that is determined by

Specularities Reduce Ambiguity of Uncalibrated Photometric Stereo

Affine Surface Reconstruction By Purposive Viewpoint Control

CEng 477 Introduction to Computer Graphics Fall 2007

Photometric stereo. Recovering the surface f(x,y) Three Source Photometric stereo: Step1. Reflectance Map of Lambertian Surface

Computer Graphics Ray Casting. Matthias Teschner

Formal ambiguities seen by analyzing the generative model, the bas-relief transform. Learning scene parameters from images

Micro-scale Stereo Photogrammetry of Skin Lesions for Depth and Colour Classification

Stereo Vision. MAN-522 Computer Vision

Basic distinctions. Definitions. Epstein (1965) familiar size experiment. Distance, depth, and 3D shape cues. Distance, depth, and 3D shape cues

3D Modeling in Teaching and Learning Geometry

Local invariant features

Investigation of Directional Filter on Kube-Pentland s 3D Surface Reflectance Model using Photometric Stereo

Midterm Exam! CS 184: Foundations of Computer Graphics! page 1 of 13!

Design, Computation and Computer Controlled Devices

International Journal of Advance Engineering and Research Development

The Role of Light to Sight

COMP 558 lecture 16 Nov. 8, 2010

SINGULARITY THEORY. International Review of Mathematics. Manchester, 4 December 2003

ü 12.1 Vectors Students should read Sections of Rogawski's Calculus [1] for a detailed discussion of the material presented in this section.

Introduction to 3D Imaging: Perceiving 3D from 2D Images

Transcription:

Event Perception and Action VI 65 Limits on the Perception of Local Shape from Shading Roderik Erens, Astrid M. L. Kappers, and Jan J. Koenderink Utrecht Biophysics Research Institute, The Netherlands Abstract. Local shape is not determined uniquely by shading. This leads to a wrong interpretation of shape by the human visual system when only shading information is present. When a cast shadow indicates the direction of the illuminant, concave and convex shapes are distinguishable, but elliptic and hyperbolic shapes are not distinguishable. Introduction Artists are very skilled in creating depth in natural images with the use of only monocular cues. Shading is generally expected to be one of the most important cues for creating this depth impression. But extracting shape from shading is not a trivial task. The only input of the visual system is the luminance data, determined by the surface orientation, the direction of the illuminant and the surface properties. Therefore the shape from shading problem is vastly underconstrained. Simplifying assumptions have to be made by the human visual system to solve this problem. Several psychophysical experiments have been performed on the perception of shape from shading (Mingo lla & Todd, 1986; Pentland, 1989). Some experiments have treated the problem of distinguishing between concave and convex shapes (Berbaum, Bever, & Sup Chung, 1984; Ramachandran, 1988), but the question of distinguishing between hyperbolic and elliptic shapes by the use of shading information alone has never been attempted before. We have conducted several experiments on the perception of quadric surfaces with shading as depth cue. Quadric surfaces describe the whole range of elliptic and hyperbolic shapes. We are interested in the performance in distinguishing different shapes. The contribution of a cast shadow, which reveals the direction of the illuminant, is also investigated. Methods Every solid shape can be described locally by patches of quadric surfaces. We used the shape index and curvedness introduced byjan Koenderink (199), to describe the quadric shape. The shape index scale represents continuously all possible quadric shapes, but can roughly be divided in a concave elliptic, a hyperbolic and a convex elliptic part (Figure 1).

66 Event Perception and Action VI o'csd i -1. -.5..5 1. Figure 1. Local shapes presented on the shape index scale. A quadric surface is described in tangent coordinates by the expression: Pixy) = ( Klx 1 + Ky) (1) where K1 and K are the principle curvatures. The shape index and curvedness are given by: S = - arctan It K1 + K K1 K () C V11(1+ K/ (3) S E 1-1,11, C [,) The shape index varies from -1 for a concave symmetric paraboloid up to 1 for a convex symmetric paraboloid. The curvedness varies from for a flat surface up to infinity for an extremely curved surface. The images are projected perspectively on a high resolution monitor and viewed monocularly from 1 m distance. The quadric surfaces are shaded diffusely, and the curvedness is 3 m-1 (think of a tennisbal). The perspective projection gives every shape a specific outline (this is not an occluding contour!), by which it can be recognized easily. Therefore, the quadric shapes are covered with a random frayed grey mask. The quadric shapes are oriented randomly around the z-axis; otherwise the principle curvatures are always aligned along the x- and y-axis. The outline and orientation of the quadric surface would be a very strong cue for recognizing the image. The direction of the illuminant was chosen randomly from four directions: 45, 135, 5, or 315 with the positive x-axis. With our setup the only cue for perceiving the shape is shading. In two experiments 3 series of images are presented for 4 s with the shape index chosen randomly. Four subjects participated in the experiment, all

Event Perception and Action VI 67 familiar with the scale of the shape index and curvedness. In the experiments the shape index scale was divided into eight equal parts. Subjects answered on the shape index scale by picking a category. First experiment In a first experiment we investigated the possibility to perceive difference between convex, concave or parabolic and hyperbolic shapes, with only shading. The light intensity for a diffusely shaded image is given by: I(x,y) = pmn. L) (4) where p is the surface albedo, and A. is the intensity of the illuminant. For a arbitrary direction of the illuminant: L = (11, 1, /3) V11+ 1 + 13 (5) Then, the light intensity for a quadric surface is given by: -K1x11 Ky1 + 13 Axy) pa / /,,/ 1 1 + 1 + 13 "/ K1 x + K1 y + 1 (6) It can be easily shown that we invert convex into concave by rotating the light source over 1. This is the well known crater illusion. It is also possible to invert hyperbolic shapes into elliptic ones by rotating the light over 9 or 7. Second experiment Information about the direction of the illuminant is needed. In real images there are several hints to knowing this direction by interreflections between objects, cast shadows, or the illuminance profile near occluding contours. In this second experiment we placed a small stick on the surface which generated a cast shadow. The direction of the illuminant (li and 1, formula 6) is visualized with the cast shadow. The influence of a cast shadow on the perception ofconvex and concave ellipsoids is often investigated (Berbaum, Bever, & Sup Chung, 1984; Ramachandran, 1988), but the influence of a cast shadow on the perception of elliptic and hyperbolic shapes is less clear and never investigated before.

68 Event Perception and Action VI The intensity distribution does not uniquely describe a certain shape. The intensity distribution will not vary so drastically for concave or convex elliptic patches. On a concave elliptic patch the intensity gradient will always point in the direction of the illuminant and on a convex elliptic patch in the opposite direction. A cast shadow will directly solve the concave-convex ambiguity. It is always possible to get on hyperbolic patches intensity patterns like on elliptic patches, for specific directions of the illuminant. Therefore, hyperbolic shapes can be interpreted just as easily as being concave or convex elliptic shapes. Results Figure shows the results of a subject for the first experiment with no cast shadow information. The eight graphs represent the eight categories. On the y- axis the score in % is plotted against the answered categories on the x-axis. Figure clearly shows that shadinginformation alone, is not enough to perceive the correct shape. Most shapes are perceived totally wrong and there is a small tendency to answer on the positive (convex) axes of the shape index scale. No difference between convex and concave elliptic and hyperbolic patches is found. Furthermore, the correlation between the answered shape index and the direction of the illuminant is checked. It seems that no specific direction of the illuminant is assumed. The influence of the cast shadow on the perception of the shape is shown in Figure 3. All concave elliptic shapes are answered on the concave side of the shape index scale and also the convex ellipticshapes are reported as convex. But the hyperbolic shapes (Figure 3, categories 3 to 6) lead to random response on the whole scale of the shape index. Apparently, it is not possible to distinguish between elliptic and hyperbolic shapes. The knowledge of the direction of the illuniinant cannot solve this ambiguity. Solving the shape from shading problem is complicated and the information of the direction of the illuminant is not enough for a unique interpretation of the intensity distribution. Discussion The concave-convex ambiguity is rather familiar and is often investigated in psychophysics (Berbaum, Bever, & Sup Chung, 1984; Ramachandran, 1988). But an arbitrary solid shape is described by hyperbolic and elliptic patches. Strangely enough, in most shape from shading experiments only I 3.

Event Perception and Action VI 69, 6 at7' 6. 1 p 5 MM1= 1. 1. a g' *6 a. 6 4111UomPolailli., 1137 6 cp' 6 4(,,...ALAIN. 3 7 too rimil_ 3 6 m. 4 a 6 INE, MEI 8 Figure. The results of the first experiment with no cast shadow. The score (%) is plotted against the answered categories. Each graph represents a different category.

7 Event Perception and Action VI clo 6 1 5 i zo LTRILT4116 1 3 4 answered 5 6 7 8 category co cn eo. co Dr co a. 6 6 "NINET1.1111k, a) 6 6 ao :11111,1114111... 3 a. 7 6 4 8 co.41.41111.11111 Figure 3. The results of the experiment with a cast shadow. The score (%) is plotted against the answered categories. Each graph represents a different category.

Event Perception and Action VI 71 elliptic shapes are used. Therefore, the impossibility to distinguish between elliptic and hyperbolic patches with shading information only has never been noticed by others. This type of ambiguity is even stronger than the concaveconvex ambiguity. With a cast shadow it is directly possible to distinguish between concave and convex shapes, but it is still impossible to distinguish between elliptic and hyperbolic patches. Finding the local shape of a diffusely shaded object seems to be complicated. Even with knowledge of the direction of the illuminant it is not possible to distinguish between elliptic and hyperbolic shapes. However, in a natural image we get a strong 3D impression. In these images there are usually much more cues to get a 3D impression. For example the occluding contour, the interreflections between surfaces, the projection of a cast shadow on a surface, and the whole morphology of hyperbolic and elliptic regions on a complicated object (Koenderink & van Doom, 1981). However, it is never investigated extensively what the contribution of each of these cues is to the perception of 3D shape. Acknowledgment. This research is supported by the SPIN project '3D-Computer Vision' of the Dutch Ministry of Economic Affairs. References Berbaum, K., Bever, T., & Sup Chung, C. (1984). Extending the perception of shape from known to unknown shading. Perception, 13, 479-488. Ramachandran, V. S. (1988). Perceiving shape from shading. Scientific American, 59(), 58-65. Mingolla, E., & Todd, J. T. (1986). Perception of solid shape from shading. Biological Cybernetics, 53, 137-151. Pentland, A. (1989). Shape information from shading: A theory about human perception. Spatial Vision, 4, 165-18. Koenderink, J. J. (199). Solid shape. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Koenderink, J. J., & van Doorn, A. J. (1981). Photometric invariants related to solid shape. Optica Acta, 7, 981-996.