When this experiment is performed, subjects find that they can always. test field. adjustable field

Similar documents
this is processed giving us: perceived color that we actually experience and base judgments upon.

The Elements of Colour

Reading. 2. Color. Emission spectra. The radiant energy spectrum. Watt, Chapter 15.

Colour Reading: Chapter 6. Black body radiators

Digital Image Processing. Introduction

Digital Image Processing COSC 6380/4393. Lecture 19 Mar 26 th, 2019 Pranav Mantini

CSE 167: Lecture #6: Color. Jürgen P. Schulze, Ph.D. University of California, San Diego Fall Quarter 2012

Spectral Color and Radiometry

Color and Shading. Color. Shapiro and Stockman, Chapter 6. Color and Machine Vision. Color and Perception

CSE 167: Lecture #6: Color. Jürgen P. Schulze, Ph.D. University of California, San Diego Fall Quarter 2011

Digital Image Processing

Why does a visual system need color? Color. Why does a visual system need color? (an incomplete list ) Lecture outline. Reading: Optional reading:

Color. Reading: Optional reading: Chapter 6, Forsyth & Ponce. Chapter 4 of Wandell, Foundations of Vision, Sinauer, 1995 has a good treatment of this.

Image Formation. Ed Angel Professor of Computer Science, Electrical and Computer Engineering, and Media Arts University of New Mexico

Announcements. Lighting. Camera s sensor. HW1 has been posted See links on web page for readings on color. Intro Computer Vision.

Lecture 12 Color model and color image processing

CS 111: Digital Image Processing Fall 2016 Midterm Exam: Nov 23, Pledge: I neither received nor gave any help from or to anyone in this exam.

Sources, Surfaces, Eyes

Introduction to color science

CHAPTER 3 COLOR MEASUREMENT USING CHROMATICITY DIAGRAM - SOFTWARE

2003 Steve Marschner 7 Light detection discrete approx. Cone Responses S,M,L cones have broadband spectral sensitivity This sum is very clearly a dot

Introduction to Computer Graphics with WebGL

Lecture 1 Image Formation.

CS 445 HW#6 Solutions

CSE 167: Introduction to Computer Graphics Lecture #6: Colors. Jürgen P. Schulze, Ph.D. University of California, San Diego Fall Quarter 2013

CS635 Spring Department of Computer Science Purdue University

Colour computer vision: fundamentals, applications and challenges. Dr. Ignacio Molina-Conde Depto. Tecnología Electrónica Univ.

(0, 1, 1) (0, 1, 1) (0, 1, 0) What is light? What is color? Terminology

Characterizing and Controlling the. Spectral Output of an HDR Display

ams AG TAOS Inc. is now The technical content of this TAOS document is still valid. Contact information:

Automated Control of The Color Rendering Index for LED RGBW Modules in Industrial Lighting

Color, Edge and Texture

Physical Color. Color Theory - Center for Graphics and Geometric Computing, Technion 2

The Display pipeline. The fast forward version. The Display Pipeline The order may vary somewhat. The Graphics Pipeline. To draw images.

Image Processing. Color

3D graphics, raster and colors CS312 Fall 2010

Color Vision. Spectral Distributions Various Light Sources

Color to Binary Vision. The assignment Irfanview: A good utility Two parts: More challenging (Extra Credit) Lighting.

Visible Color. 700 (red) 580 (yellow) 520 (green)

MODELING LED LIGHTING COLOR EFFECTS IN MODERN OPTICAL ANALYSIS SOFTWARE LED Professional Magazine Webinar 10/27/2015

SPECTRAL ANALYSIS OF THE COLOR OF SOME PIGMENTS

Pop Quiz 1 [10 mins]

Fall 2015 Dr. Michael J. Reale

CSE 167: Lecture #7: Color and Shading. Jürgen P. Schulze, Ph.D. University of California, San Diego Fall Quarter 2011

Lecture 1. Computer Graphics and Systems. Tuesday, January 15, 13

(Equation 24.1: Index of refraction) We can make sense of what happens in Figure 24.1

Topics: Chromaticity, white point, and quality metrics

Colorimetric Quantities and Laws

Geometric Invariants Under Illuminant Transformations

Game Programming. Bing-Yu Chen National Taiwan University

The exam begins at 2:40pm and ends at 4:00pm. You must turn your exam in when time is announced or risk not having it accepted.

All forms of EM waves travel at the speed of light in a vacuum = 3.00 x 10 8 m/s This speed is constant in air as well

COLOR FIDELITY OF CHROMATIC DISTRIBUTIONS BY TRIAD ILLUMINANT COMPARISON. Marcel P. Lucassen, Theo Gevers, Arjan Gijsenij

CS770/870 Spring 2017 Color and Shading

Computer Graphics. Bing-Yu Chen National Taiwan University The University of Tokyo

LED Evenement 2014 Spectroscopy - Straylight. Avantes BV Apeldoorn, The Netherlands

CS681 Computational Colorimetry

Chapter 1. Light and color

Lighting. Camera s sensor. Lambertian Surface BRDF

LECTURE 37: Ray model of light and Snell's law

What is it? How does it work? How do we use it?

Light, Lenses, Mirrors

Chapter 5 Extraction of color and texture Comunicação Visual Interactiva. image labeled by cluster index

Image Formation. Camera trial #1. Pinhole camera. What is an Image? Light and the EM spectrum The H.V.S. and Color Perception

Color. Computational Photography MIT Feb. 14, 2006 Bill Freeman and Fredo Durand

Chapter 6 Color Image Processing

CS452/552; EE465/505. Color Display Issues

CIE Colour Chart-based Video Monitoring

Color Matching with Amplitude Not Left Out

Lecture #13. Point (pixel) transformations. Neighborhood processing. Color segmentation

specular diffuse reflection.

Color Image Processing

Reprint (R30) Accurate Chromaticity Measurements of Lighting Components. Reprinted with permission from Craig J. Coley The Communications Repair depot

Color Appearance in Image Displays. O Canada!

f. (5.3.1) So, the higher frequency means the lower wavelength. Visible part of light spectrum covers the range of wavelengths from

Illumination and Shading

Understanding Variability

Integer Operations. Summer Packet 7 th into 8 th grade 1. Name = = = = = 6.

Lecture #2: Color and Linear Algebra pt.1

Conceptual Physics 11 th Edition

HW Chapter 20 Q 2,3,4,5,6,10,13 P 1,2,3. Chapter 20. Classic and Modern Optics. Dr. Armen Kocharian

4. A bulb has a luminous flux of 2400 lm. What is the luminous intensity of the bulb?

E (sensor) is given by; Object Size

10.4 Interference in Thin Films

What is Color and How is It Measured?

SPECIAL TECHNIQUES-II

Diffraction. PHYS 1301 F98 Prof. T.E. Coan Last edit 6 Aug 98. Introduction

Textbook Reference: Physics (Wilson, Buffa, Lou): Chapter 24

INTRODUCTION. Slides modified from Angel book 6e

Abridged Spectral Fluorescence Imaging An innovative Graduate Student Research Report

ELEC Dr Reji Mathew Electrical Engineering UNSW

Announcements. Camera Calibration. Thin Lens: Image of Point. Limits for pinhole cameras. f O Z

ECE-161C Color. Nuno Vasconcelos ECE Department, UCSD (with thanks to David Forsyth)

Grade 5. Massachusetts Curriculum Framework for Mathematics 48

Analysis of spectrophotometer specular performance using goniometric information

Planting the Seeds Exploring Cubic Functions

4.5 Images Formed by the Refraction of Light

Reading. 4. Color. Outline. The radiant energy spectrum. Suggested: w Watt (2 nd ed.), Chapter 14. Further reading:

Application of CIE with Associated CRI-based Colour Rendition Properties

COMP3421. Global Lighting Part 2: Radiosity

Transcription:

COLORIMETRY In photometry a lumen is a lumen, no matter what wavelength or wavelengths of light are involved. But it is that combination of wavelengths that produces the sensation of color, one of the most significant properties of light. While photometry deals only with the amount of light, colorimetry deals with the quantitative specification of the color of light. Colorimetric principles have applications in photography, television, printing, etc. COLOR MATCHING Colorimetry is based on the fact that most colors may be matched by superimposing light from three sources, typically a red, green, and blue source. These are called the primary colors or just primaries. Extending the notion of color matching with a little mathematical chicanery it is, in fact, possible to match any color of light with three primaries. Suppose, for example, we have projectors which produce spectrally pure 700 nm red light, 546 nm green light, and 436 nm blue light. If we project light so that the illuminance of the sources on a white receiving screen are 100 lm/sq m of red light, 459 lm/sq m of green light, and 6 lm/sq m of blue light, as in the diagram below, we'll find that the area where all three sources overlap has an illuminance of 565 lm/sq m of white light. blue magenta white greenblue red yellow green Figure 1. Overlapping red, green, and blue lights produce white light. Colorimetry, page 1 W. F. Long, 1996

If we were to increase the illuminance of 700 nm red light 100 lm/sq m to 200 lm sq/m, the superimposition of the three lights on a white screen would be pink or a desaturated red, that is, a red with white mixed in, pink, if you like. Its illuminance would be 665 lm/sq m. The hue or dominant wavelength of the combination would be that of 700 nm red light. Pure spectral colors contain no white component and are said to be saturated. Spectral colors can be produced by a monochromator which uses a prism or grating. As long as the proportions of the color components are kept the same, the hue and saturation of the combination remains the same, only the illuminance changing. Overlapping 200 lm/sq m of red light, 918 lm/sq m of green light, and 12 lm/sq m of blue light would produce the same white light as before, but with 1130 lm/sq m illuminance, twice as much as before. Likewise, 100 lm/sq m of red light, 230 lm/sq m of green light, and 3 lm/sq m of blue light would produce the same pink light as in the example above, but with the illuminance halved to 333 lm/sq m. The experiment underlying all of colorimetry is that of color matching. A typical field of view in a color matching experiment is shown in figure 2. The task of the experimental subject is to adjust the luminances of red, green, and blue lights on the left side of the field to match the colored field at the right. test field adjustable field Figure 2. In a colorimetry experiment a subject would adjust the luminances of red, green, and blue on the left hand side of the field to match a colored field on the right. The field typically subtends 2 of arc at the eye. When this experiment is performed, subjects find that they can always Colorimetry, page 2 W. F. Long, 1996

match the luminance and hue of the test stimulus, but often they cannot match its saturation. In such cases the hue has been matched using two of the primaries. To complete the match, the experimenter allows the subject to add the third primary to the test field until a match has been obtained. This is equivalent to adding a negative luminance to the adjustable field. If, for example, a match has been obtained using 100 lx of red light, 50 lx of green light, with 30 lx of blue light added to the test field, that is mathematically equivalent to matching the test field with 100 lx of red light, 50 lx of green light, and -30 lx of blue light. The negative luminance has a purely mathematical meaning. On paper we can add together primaries with combinations of positive and negative luminances mathematically which correspond to no real physical color. These are the so-called imaginary colors, and no one has or ever will actually see them. Unfortunately, these imaginary colors have a perversely important role in the usual formulation of colorimetry, as we shall see. In colorimetry it is not very convenient to quantify stimuli in the usual photometric units based on luminous flux. Instead the luminance, L, of a stimulus is given in terms of tristimulus values R, G, and B by the equation L(lx)=R+4.5907G+0.0601B when using the 700 nm, 546 nm, and 436 nm primaries we discussed above. This serves to quantize the units of the different primaries. This particular quantization gives white light whenever R=G=B. We can think of tristimulus values as a three dimensional vector (R,G,B). In this notation the white of our first example would have components (100,100,100). The pink color in the example would be (200,100,100). A spectral 700 nm red of 300 lx luminance would be represented by (300,0,0). If we were to double the luminance of a stimulus with tristimulus values (R,G,B), the resultant stimulus would have tristimulus values 2(R,G,B)=(2R,2G,2B). If we were to combine stimuli with tristimulus values (R 1,G 1,B 1 ) and (R 2,G 2,B 2 ), the combination would have tristimulus values (R,G,B)=(R 1,G 1,B 1 )+(R 2,G 2,B 2 )=(R 1 +R 2,G 1 +G 2,B 1 +B 2 ). Thus the tristimulus value vector obeys the usual laws of vector addition. The color matching functions or distribution coefficients r λ, g λ, and b λ are special tristimulus values which apply to a spectral source of wave length λ Colorimetry, page 3 W. F. Long, 1996

and unit energy.1 The tristimulus values for a spectral source of power P λ is (R,G,B)=P λ (r λ,g λ,b λ ). The distribution functions determined from color matching experiments are plotted in figure 3. Note how the values of all the curves but that associated with the relevant primary vanish at 700 nm, 546 nm, and 436 nm. And also note the negative values for many of the distribution coefficients. 0.4 0.3 rgb distribution coefficients b λ 565 nm r λ 0.2 436 nm g λ 0.1 700 nm 0.0-0.1 300 400 500 600 wavelength (nm) 700 800 Figure 3. The distribution coefficients in the rgb color specification system. Color specification is usually concerned only with the relative proportions of the three primaries. The chromaticity coordinates r, g, and b specify color without specifying luminance. They are defined in terms of the tristimulus values as follows: r=r/(r+g+b), g=g/(r+g+b), b=b/(r+g+b). Since r+g+b=1 (why?), only two chromaticity coordinates are independent. Like the tristimulus values, the chromaticity coordinates may be Colorimetry, page 4 W. F. Long, 1996

represented as a vector (r,g,b). The chromaticity coordinates of white are (1/3, 1/3, 1/3). The chromaticity coordinates of our pink example are (1/2, 1/4, 1/4). The chromaticity coordinates of a 700 nm light of any luminance would be (1, 0, 0). Two assumptions underly all of this: (1) hue and saturation, the color information about a light source, are independent of the luminance of the light source; (2) light sources obey Abney's law, that is lumens add algebraically regardless of the colors of the contributing light sources. Though there has been argument about the validity of these assumptions, we will continue to accept them here. THE CIE COLORIMETRIC SYSTEM The results of color matching experiments done with one set of primaries may be converted to those done with another set of primaries by using a linear transformation, a so-called linear transform which represents the chromaticity vector in a new coordinate system. In this way, for example, David Wright was able to compare his distribution coefficients taken with 650 nm, 530 nm, and 460 nm primaries with J. Guild's data taken using 700 nm, 546 nm, and 436 nm primaries. In 1931 the Comission Internationale d'eclairage (CIE) adopted a standard system for color specification. The CIE, in its wisdom, chose to use imaginary primaries designated X, Y, Z. (Here's where those darned imaginary colors come in.) The tristimulus values in the CIE system are (X,Y,Z). The chromaticity coordinates are (x,y,z), and they are related to the tristimulus values as in the rgb system: x=x/(x+y+z), y=y/(x+y+z), z=z/(x+y+z). The distribution coefficients are x λ, y λ, and z λ. These are plotted in figure 4 and tabulated in table 1. 1Distribution coefficients are more commonly notated as a variable over which there is a bar. Colorimetry, page 5 W. F. Long, 1996

2.0 z λ 1.5 CIE Distribution Coefficients 1.0 y λ x λ 0.5 0.0 350 450 550 wavelength (nm) 650 750 Figure 4. Distribution coefficients in the CIE system. Note the familiar shape of the y λ curve. It is, in fact, an old friend the spectral sensitivity function v λ. Making the distribution function of the Y color the same as the spectral sensitivity function is only one of the mathematical miracles of the CIE system. Another is that the distribution functions are all positive. The chromaticity coordinates of all real light sources are positive. And the system still allows white light to have chromaticity coordinates (1/3, 1/3, 1/3). All that has been sacrificed in all this mathematical elegance is any physical intuition. While many think the CIE system a clever mess, it's standard and we're stuck with it! The chromaticity coordinates of the spectral colors may be calculated from the distribution functions remembering that the distribution functions are just tristimulus values. So for a spectral color of wavelength λ, the color coordinates are x=x λ /(x λ +y λ +z λ ), y=y λ /(x λ +y λ +z λ ), z=z λ /(x λ +y λ +z λ ). Colorimetry, page 6 W. F. Long, 1996

λ x λ y λ z λ 380 0.001 0.000 0.006 390 0.004 0.000 0.020 400 0.014 0.000 0.068 410 0.044 0.001 0.207 420 0.134 0.004 0.646 430 0.283 0.012 1.386 440 0.348 0.023 1.747 450 0.336 0.038 1.772 460 0.291 0.060 1.669 470 0.195 0.061 1.288 480 0.096 0.139 0.813 490 0.032 0.208 0.465 500 0.005 0.323 0.272 510 0.009 0.503 0.158 520 0.063 0.710 0.078 530 0.166 0.862 0.042 540 0.290 0.954 0.020 550 0.433 0.995 0.009 560 0.594 0.995 0.004 570 0.762 0.952 0.002 580 0.916 0.870 0.002 590 1.026 0.757 0.001 600 1.062 0.631 0.001 610 1.002 0.503 0.000 620 0.854 0.381 0.000 630 0.642 0.265 0.000 640 0.448 0.175 0.000 650 0.284 0.107 0.000 660 0.165 0.061 0.000 670 0.087 0.032 0.000 680 0.047 0.017 0.000 690 0.023 0.008 0.000 700 0.011 0.004 0.000 710 0.006 0.002 0.000 720 0.003 0.001 0.000 Table 1. Distribution coefficients in the CIE system given to three places. For 550 nm spectral light, for example, the chromaticity coordinates are x=0.433/(0.433+0.995+0.009)=0.30, y=0.995/(0.433+0.995+0.009)=0.69, z=0.009/(0.433+0.995+0.009)=0.01. These, of course, sum to one, as they must. Colorimetry, page 7 W. F. Long, 1996

THE CHROMATICITY DIAGRAM It is useful in visualizing the CIE system and in doing certain practical calculations to plot the chromaticity coordinates of the spectral colors on a diagram. Since only two of the chromaticity coordinates are independent, we plot only the x and y chromaticity coordinates to get the diagram of figure 5. 1.0 0.9 0.8 520 530 CIE chromaticity diagram 540 0.7 0.6 510 bluegreen 550 green 560 570 y 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 500 490 blue ultramarine W purple 580 yellow orange 590 600 610 red 700 620 0.1 0.0 470 480 violet 400 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 x 1.0 Figure 5. The CIE chromaticity diagram. The spectral colors lie on a curved, convex locus. In the chromaticity diagram, the wavelength (in nm) of each spectral source is written next to the circle indicating its chromaticity coordinates. The usual color names associated with wavelengths are in table 2. The shorter wavelengths are widely spaced on the convex portion of the locus to the left. Longer wavelengths are closer together along an almost straight locus to the right. The points corresponding to wavelengths at the extreme ends of the visual spectrum are crowded together. The point corresponding to white light with chromaticity coordinates (1/3, 1/3) is designated W. Colorimetry, page 8 W. F. Long, 1996

wavelength range color name greater than 620 nm red 620 nm to 592 nm orange 592 nm to 578 nm yellow 578 nm to 513 nm green 513 nm to 500 nm blue-green 500 nm t o 464 nm blue 464 nm to 446 nm ultramarine less than 446 nm violet Table 2. Color names corresponding to wavelengths. A useful feature of this sort of representation is that the chromaticity coordinates (x,y) of a color achieved by combining two light sources represented by chromaticity coordinates (x 1,y 1 ) and (x 2,y 2 ) lies along the line connecting (x 1,y 1 ) and (x 2,y 2 ) on the chromaticity diagram, as shown in figure 6. (x, y ) 2 2 S S 2 (x, y ) 1 1 S 1 (x, y) Figure 6. When two colors S 1 and S 2 are mixed, their resultant S lies at the center of gravity between their points on the chromaticity diagram. If S 1 has luminance L 1 and S 2 has luminance L 2, then S 1 S/S 2 S=L 1 /L 2. Thus the chromaticity of the combination lies closest to the chromaticity coordinate of the stronger source. If both sources have equal luminance, the combination lies halfway between. If one source has zero luminance, the chromaticity coordinate of the combination coincides with the other source. Thus the combination lies at the center of gravity of the two sources. Colorimetry, page 9 W. F. Long, 1996

The colors achieved by combining colors at short and long wavelength extremes of the visible spectrum are on the straight line portion of the spectral locus of the CIE diagram. These colors are purples. We can achieve the same color an infinite number of ways. In figure 7, for example, the color represented by point S can be obtained by combining S 1 and S 2 or S 3 and S 4 in the proper proportions. Both combinations will look the same to an observer, though the spectral composition of their sources are quite different. These are called metamers. S 3 S S 2 S 4 S 1 Figure 7. The color represented by point S may be achieved by combining sources S 1 and S 2 or S 3 and S 4 in the proper proportions. The two combinations are metamers which appear the same to observers. If we add white light to a spectral source, the combination lies along the line connecting the chromaticity of the spectral source along the spectral locus and the point W. The closer the point to W the more desaturated the color. If λ is the point on the spectral locus and S a source point somewhere within the spectral locus, the excitation purity, p e, of the source is p e =WS/Wλ. The dominant hue of a source S may be determined from the intersection of the line through W and S with the spectral locus. In figure 5, the dominant color names associated with dominant hues are indicated in wedges centered at point W. Any color with chromaticity coordinates within one of these wedges has the indicated color name, the points closer to W being more desaturated. Colorimetry, page 10 W. F. Long, 1996

For every color there is a complementary color which can be combined with it in suitable proportions to produce white. Clearly the line connecting complementary colors must intersect W (figure 8). λ 1 W λ 2 Figure 8. The line connecting complementary colors λ 1 and λ 2 passes through W, the point corresponding to white light. CALCULATIONS IN THE CIE SYSTEM We can use the theory presented above to find the color, chromaticity coordinates, saturation, and complements of a light source or combination of light sources. In this section we'll give some examples. Example 1. Find the chromaticity coordinates and color of an "equal energy" source, a source for which P λ =const. Solution: Since the chromaticity coordinates are relative values independent of the radiance of a source, we can, for simplicity let P λ =1. Recalling that the distribution functions are the tristimulus values of unit energy sources, the tristimulus values of the combination of sources is X=Σ(x λ P λ ), Y=Σ(y λ P λ ), Z=Σ(z λ P λ ). (Technically we should integrate over the energy distribution, but in practice we have to use Simpson's rule which gives the sums above.) The chromaticity coordinates can be calculated from the tristimulus coefficients. We can lay out our calculation on a spreadsheet as shown below: Colorimetry, page 11 W. F. Long, 1996

λ x λ y λ z λ P λ x P λ λ y P λ λ z P λ λ 380 0.001 0.000 0.006 1.000 0.001 0.000 0.006 390 0.004 0.000 0.020 1.000 0.004 0.000 0.020 400 0.014 0.000 0.068 1.000 0.014 0.000 0.068 410 0.044 0.001 0.207 1.000 0.044 0.001 0.207 420 0.134 0.004 0.646 1.000 0.134 0.004 0.646 430 0.283 0.012 1.386 1.000 0.283 0.012 1.386 440 0.348 0.023 1.747 1.000 0.348 0.023 1.747 450 0.336 0.038 1.772 1.000 0.336 0.038 1.772 460 0.291 0.060 1.669 1.000 0.291 0.060 1.669 470 0.195 0.061 1.288 1.000 0.195 0.061 1.288 480 0.096 0.139 0.813 1.000 0.096 0.139 0.813 490 0.032 0.208 0.465 1.000 0.032 0.208 0.465 500 0.005 0.323 0.272 1.000 0.005 0.323 0.272 510 0.009 0.503 0.158 1.000 0.009 0.503 0.158 520 0.063 0.710 0.078 1.000 0.063 0.710 0.078 530 0.166 0.862 0.042 1.000 0.166 0.862 0.042 540 0.290 0.954 0.020 1.000 0.290 0.954 0.020 550 0.433 0.995 0.009 1.000 0.433 0.995 0.009 560 0.594 0.995 0.004 1.000 0.594 0.995 0.004 570 0.762 0.952 0.002 1.000 0.762 0.952 0.002 580 0.916 0.870 0.002 1.000 0.916 0.870 0.002 590 1.026 0.757 0.001 1.000 1.026 0.757 0.001 600 1.062 0.631 0.001 1.000 1.062 0.631 0.001 610 1.002 0.503 0.000 1.000 1.002 0.503 0.000 620 0.854 0.381 0.000 1.000 0.854 0.381 0.000 630 0.642 0.265 0.000 1.000 0.642 0.265 0.000 640 0.448 0.175 0.000 1.000 0.448 0.175 0.000 650 0.284 0.107 0.000 1.000 0.284 0.107 0.000 660 0.165 0.061 0.000 1.000 0.165 0.061 0.000 670 0.087 0.032 0.000 1.000 0.087 0.032 0.000 680 0.047 0.017 0.000 1.000 0.047 0.017 0.000 690 0.023 0.008 0.000 1.000 0.023 0.008 0.000 700 0.011 0.004 0.000 1.000 0.011 0.004 0.000 710 0.006 0.002 0.000 1.000 0.006 0.002 0.000 720 0.003 0.001 0.000 1.000 0.003 0.001 0.000 X=Σ(x P ) λ λ Y=Σ(y P ) λ λ Z=Σ(z P ) λ λ 10.676 10.654 10.676 x=x/(x+y+z)y=y/(x+y+z) z=z/(x+y+z) 0.33 0.33 0.33 Thus the chromaticity coordinates of an equal energy source are (0.33,0.33,0.33). This, however, is point W, the location of a white source, so the equal energy source is white. Colorimetry, page 12 W. F. Long, 1996

Example 2. Find the chromaticity coordinates, color, saturation, and complementary color for white light from an equal energy source transmitted through a filter with spectral transmittance shown in the graph below. 1.0 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 τ λ 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.0 350 400 450 500 550 600 wavelength (nm) 650 700 750 Figure 9. The spectral transmittance of a Kalichrome lens. Solution: For light from an equal energy light source going through a filter with spectral transmittant τ λ P λ τ λ X=Σ(x λ τ λ ), Y=Σ(y λ τ λ ), Z=Σ(z λ τ λ ). We can calculate tristimulus values from the distribution coefficients as we did in example 1, using the spreadsheet below. The values of spectral transmittance are read from the graph above. Colorimetry, page 13 W. F. Long, 1996

λ x λ y λ z λ τ λ x τ λ λ y τ λ λ z τ λ λ 380 0.001 0.000 0.006 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 390 0.004 0.000 0.020 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 400 0.014 0.000 0.068 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 410 0.044 0.001 0.207 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 420 0.134 0.004 0.646 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 430 0.283 0.012 1.386 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 440 0.348 0.023 1.747 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 450 0.336 0.038 1.772 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 460 0.291 0.060 1.669 0.03 0.01 0.00 0.05 470 0.195 0.061 1.288 0.06 0.01 0.00 0.08 480 0.096 0.139 0.813 0.12 0.01 0.02 0.10 490 0.032 0.208 0.465 0.18 0.01 0.04 0.08 500 0.005 0.323 0.272 0.27 0.00 0.09 0.07 510 0.009 0.503 0.158 0.38 0.00 0.19 0.06 520 0.063 0.710 0.078 0.81 0.05 0.58 0.06 530 0.166 0.862 0.042 0.82 0.14 0.71 0.03 540 0.290 0.954 0.020 0.83 0.24 0.79 0.02 550 0.433 0.995 0.009 0.84 0.36 0.84 0.01 560 0.594 0.995 0.004 0.85 0.50 0.85 0.00 570 0.762 0.952 0.002 0.85 0.65 0.81 0.00 580 0.916 0.870 0.002 0.85 0.78 0.74 0.00 590 1.026 0.757 0.001 0.85 0.87 0.64 0.00 600 1.062 0.631 0.001 0.85 0.90 0.54 0.00 610 1.002 0.503 0.000 0.85 0.85 0.43 0.00 620 0.854 0.381 0.000 0.85 0.73 0.32 0.00 630 0.642 0.265 0.000 0.85 0.55 0.23 0.00 640 0.448 0.175 0.000 0.85 0.38 0.15 0.00 650 0.284 0.107 0.000 0.85 0.24 0.09 0.00 660 0.165 0.061 0.000 0.85 0.14 0.05 0.00 670 0.087 0.032 0.000 0.85 0.07 0.03 0.00 680 0.047 0.017 0.000 0.85 0.04 0.01 0.00 690 0.023 0.008 0.000 0.85 0.02 0.01 0.00 700 0.011 0.004 0.000 0.85 0.01 0.00 0.00 710 0.006 0.002 0.000 0.85 0.01 0.00 0.00 720 0.003 0.001 0.000 0.85 0.00 0.00 0.00 X=Σ(x τ ) λ λ Y=Σ(y τ ) λ λ Z=Σ(z τ ) λ λ 7.58 8.14 0.57 x=x/(x+y+z) y=y/(x+y+z) z=z/(x+y+z) 0.47 0.50 0.04 So the chromaticity coordinates are, finally, (0.47,0.50,0.04). The x and y coordinates on the CIE chromaticity diagram are at the gray box in the figure below, labelled S. From its position on the diagram, it is clear that the color of the filter is a quite saturated green yellow. This figures since the spectral transmittance curve is that of a Kalichrome filter, the yellow lens material used in the classic "shooter's glasses" favored by sportsmen. Colorimetry, page 14 W. F. Long, 1996

1.0 0.9 0.8 520 530 CIE chromaticity diagram 540 0.7 0.6 510 bluegreen 550 green 560 570 y 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 500 490 blue ultramarine W S purple 580 yellow orange 590 600 610 red 700 620 0.1 0.0 470 480 violet 400 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 x 1.0 Figure 10. The gray square is at the chromaticity coordinates of the Kalichrome lens of example 2. The open squares are at the chromaticity of the two stimuli of example 3 with the open circle at the chromaticity coordinates of the combination of these two stimuli. Drawing a line from WS and extending it to the spectral locus, we see the dominant hue is 576 nm. Extending the line to the opposite side of the spectral locus, we complementary color is 472 nm blue. Taking the ratio of the line segment WS to the distance from W to the spectral locus gives the excitation purity, about 90%. Colorimetry, page 15 W. F. Long, 1996

Example 3. An observer sees a stimulus composed of 20 ft L of blue light with chromaticity coordinates (0.10, 0.40, 0.50) and 60 ft L of green light with chromaticity coordinates (0.30, 0.50, 0.20). What are the luminance, color coordinates, and dominant hue of the stimulus? Solution: By Abney's law, the luminance is simply 80 ft L. We can obtain the color coordinates of the combination graphically or algebraically. Algebraically note that the tristimulus value of each color component is its luminance times its chromaticity vector. Summing these tristimulus values to get the tristimulus vector of the stimulus (X,Y,Z)=20(0.10, 0.40, 0.50)+60(0.30, 0.50, 0.20)=(20, 38, 22). The chromaticity coordinates of the combined stimuli is then (x,y,z)=[1/(20+38+22)](20, 38, 22)=(0.25, 0.47, 0.27). The chromaticity coordinates of the two stimuli are plotted with hollow squares on the diagram above. The resultant is plotted as a hollow circle. As predicted by the center of gravity rule, the circle lies on the line connecting the two stimuli, one fourth the way from the stronger of the two stimuli (why?). From its location on the CIE diagram, the resultant is green, nearly blue-green. ADDITIVE AND SUBTRACTIVE COLOR SYSTEMS The discussion above dealt with additive color systems in which lights are superimposed. Many practical systems deal with subtractive color systems, i.e. the mixing of paints or other tints. In an additive system, combined complementary colors produce white light. In a subtractive system they produce black. With appropriate adjustments, the colorimetry systems above may be applied to either kind of system. Colorimetry, page 16 W. F. Long, 1996

CONCLUSION Colorimetry is essentially a descriptive system. It summarizes the results of certain color matching experiments and allows us to apply those results to a number of real-life situations. But colorimetry doesn't explain the sensation of color or how the eye and brain process color information. It merely provides a means of quantifying color information. Colorimetry, page 17 W. F. Long, 1996